After providing all the funding for The Brain from Top to Bottom for over 10 years, the CIHR Institute of Neurosciences, Mental Health and Addiction informed us that because of budget cuts, they were going to be forced to stop sponsoring us as of March 31st, 2013.

We have approached a number of organizations, all of which have recognized the value of our work. But we have not managed to find the funding we need. We must therefore ask our readers for donations so that we can continue updating and adding new content to The Brain from Top to Bottom web site and blog.

Please, rest assured that we are doing our utmost to continue our mission of providing the general public with the best possible information about the brain and neuroscience in the original spirit of the Internet: the desire to share information free of charge and with no adverstising.

Whether your support is moral, financial, or both, thank you from the bottom of our hearts!

Bruno Dubuc, Patrick Robert, Denis Paquet, and Al Daigen




Tuesday, 22 September 2015
Seeing without knowing it : the strange phenomenon of blindsight

This week I’d like to share some information about the fascinating phenomenon of blindsight, along with links to other articles on this subject. People can become blind as the result of damage to the retina, but in rarer cases blindness can occur when both sides of the visual cortex are damaged. The latter situation can lead to the strange phenomenon known as blindsight or unconscious vision, in which people believe that they can’t see anything, but can nevertheless correctly identify the positions of objects in space. If you ask these people where a certain object or point of light is, they will say they don’t know, but if you insist and ask them to take a chance, guess, and point in the direction where they think the object or light might be, most of the time, they’ll point in the right direction.

Dutch neuroscientist Beatrice de Gelder has shown that such individuals can even avoid objects in their path as they walk down a hallway.

In other experiments, researchers have used transcranial magnetic simulation to temporarily blind people with normal sight by disrupting the electrical activity of their occipital cortex. In these cases too, the researchers found, the subjects still had some residual blindsight.

In the French-language podcast to which the last link below will take you, French neuropsychologist Sylvie Chokron talks about her team’s work with patients who have been blinded by brain lesions. Chokron has successfully taken advantage of the brain’s great plasticity to train such patients to make part of their unconscious vision conscious. Here’s how she describes her work at one point in the podcast:

[translation] When the subjects tell us that they see nothing, we ask them to put what they can’t see into words—for example, “What kind of nothing? A red nothing? A blue nothing?” More often than not, they answer correctly. And as we continue to train them, to stimulate and even overstimulate their blindsight and make them name these objects that they can’t see, these patients recover their conscious vision.

Blindsight can also tell us things about emotions, and in particular whether they are perceived consciously or not. When patients with blindness due to brain lesions are shown pictures of faces expressing various emotions, such as joy, fear, or anger, even though these patients will say that they can’t see anything, they will have physiological reactions (such as dilation of the pupils, perspiration, and changes in heart rate) which reveal that unconsciously, they are perceiving the emotional content of these pictures correctly. And when these subjects’ facial muscles are monitored, it turns out that if a picture shows a joyful expression, the muscles involved in smiling will start to contract, but if it shows a fearful expression, the muscles involved in frowning will do so, which suggests that the subjects somehow understand what kind of emotion is depicted. Also, when asked what they see, these individuals answer “nothing”, but when asked whether, if this picture showed an emotion, it would be joy or fear, they usually answer correctly.

Taken as a whole, these findings suggest that even in those of us with normal vision, part of that vision, and part of our emotional responses to the things we see every day, is unconscious, and that we probably perceive many of the features of the world around us—simple shapes, volumes, certain movements, certain colours, emotions expressed by subtle facial expressions —without even realizing it.

So just which parts of the brain provide this ability to see and to respond to things that we don’t know that we have seen? Basically, subcortical structures such as the superior colliculus or the pulvinar, which send projections directly to various parts of the visual cortex without passing through the primary visual cortex (V1). For some nice diagrams of the pathways involved, follow the third link below, to an article published in the journal Nature Reviews Neuroscience in September 2010.

i_lien Vision aveugle
i_lien Le lien qui nous rattache aux autres (5)
a_lien Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals
i_lien Unconscious Vision
i_lien Le cerveau, organe vertigineux – 3e partie

The Senses | Comments Closed


If you have a comment, please e-mail it to me, and I will post it here.